Skip to main content

Atypical multisensory integration in Niemann-Pick type C disease – towards potential biomarkers

Abstract

Background

Niemann-Pick type C (NPC) is an autosomal recessive disease in which cholesterol and glycosphingolipids accumulate in lysosomes due to aberrant cell-transport mechanisms. It is characterized by progressive and ultimately terminal neurological disease, but both pre-clinical studies and direct human trials are underway to test the safety and efficacy of cholesterol clearing compounds, with good success already observed in animal models. Key to assessing the effectiveness of interventions in patients, however, is the development of objective neurobiological outcome measures. Multisensory integration mechanisms present as an excellent candidate since they necessarily rely on the fidelity of long-range neural connections between the respective sensory cortices (e.g. the auditory and visual systems).

Methods

A simple way to test integrity of the multisensory system is to ask whether individuals respond faster to the occurrence of a bisensory event than they do to the occurrence of either of the unisensory constituents alone. Here, we presented simple auditory, visual, and audio-visual stimuli in random sequence. Participants responded as fast as possible with a button push. One 11-year-old and two 14-year-old boys with NPC participated in the experiment and their results were compared to those of 35 age-matched neurotypical boys.

Results

Reaction times (RTs) to the stimuli when presented simultaneously were significantly faster than when they were presented alone in the neurotypical children, a facilitation that could not be accounted for by probability summation, as evidenced by violation of the so-called ‘race’ model. In stark contrast, the NPC boys showed no such speeding, despite the fact that their unisensory RTs fell within the distribution of RTs observed in the neurotypicals.

Conclusions

These results uncover a previously undescribed deficit in multisensory integrative abilities in NPC, with implications for ongoing treatment of the clinical symptoms of these children. They also suggest that multisensory processes may represent a good candidate biomarker against which to test the efficacy of therapeutic interventions.

Background

Niemann-Pick type C (NPC) disease is a rare progressive lysosomal storage disorder caused by mutations in either the NPC1 or NPC2 gene, with about 95% of cases attributable to the former [1],[2]. Individuals with NPC cannot properly metabolize cholesterol and other lipids which accumulate in the brain and in visceral organs (e.g. liver and spleen), ultimately causing cell dysfunction and organ system failure. Although NPC1 and NPC2 proteins are expressed ubiquitously, brain tissue is the most severely affected, resulting in widespread intraneuronal storage of cholesterol and glycosphingolipids that ultimately results in massive neurodegeneration [3]-[6]. While appearing relatively typical during the early stages of the disease, over time NPC children develop vertical gaze palsy, motor system impairment, learning difficulties and clumsiness, as well as seizures [7]-[9]. Documented changes in brain include ectopic dendrite growth, altered synaptic connectivity affecting cortical pyramidal neurons, axonal degeneration, myelin loss, gliosis and the formation of neurofibrillary tangles similar to Alzheimer's disease [10],[11]. Neuronal death is prominent in some brain regions such as the cerebellum where Purkinje cells selectively die, undoubtedly contributing to the clinically-evident motor system dysfunction [5],[10],[12]. Effective treatments are limited, although promising clinical trials are underway based on results in animal models of NPC [11],[13],[14].

Key to advancing new treatments for this and related lysosomal diseases with neural involvement is the development of objective biomarkers of neurological function against which the efficacy of new drugs can be tested in human patients. Our work and that of others has demonstrated the essential role that multisensory integration (MSI) plays in typical perception and cognition [15]-[24]. Because inputs from the various senses (e.g., the auditory, visual and somatosensory systems) initially arrive into widely separated regions of the neocortex, MSI must involve ongoing communication between relatively far-flung cortical regions, although it may well be initiated even earlier in the hierarchy within nuclei of the thalamus [25]. In this sense, probing multisensory functioning provides an excellent assay of inter-regional communication, and the fidelity of the multisensory system must at least in part be a function of the integrity of long-range neural connectivity. For this reason we expected measures of MSI to provide a sensitive metric of neural dysfunction in NPC disease. What's more, MSI processes show a prolonged period of neuroplasticity, with continued development of these abilities seen into the late teenage years [22],[26]. As such, measures of MSI may provide useful biomarkers against which to test the impact of treatment on brain function.

A straightforward way to measure multisensory integration is to compare reaction times (RT) to unisensory and multisensory events during a simple speeded response task. It has been firmly established that adults react more quickly to multisensory than unisensory inputs [21],[27]-[30]. For such behavioral facilitation to be unequivocally attributed to multisensory integration, this speeding up must exceed what is predicted due to the mere presence of a redundant signal (i.e. two inputs). That is, when two stimulus copies are presented simultaneously, even if both were to be processed entirely independently in the brain, one would still expect to see a speeding up of responses since there is increased likelihood that either of the two stimuli will yield a fast reaction-time relative to just one input. This is often referred to as the Redundant Signals Effect (RSE), and its presence does not, of itself, necessarily point to integration effects. The so-called “race model” is applied to test for the presence of true multisensory effects, by assessing whether responses to multisensory inputs are faster than the fastest possible responses produced by the unisensory conditions [31]-[33]. This is achieved by comparing the probabilities of making fast responses during multisensory events to those during unisensory events. The race model is said to be violated whenever the cumulative probability (CP) of a response at a given latency for the multisensory condition is greater than the sum of the CPs from each of the unisensory conditions. When the race model is violated, it is taken to be a strong indication that the inputs from the two different senses are interacting (in a non-additive way) to produce the speeding of the responses. Work from our laboratory suggests that this metric of MSI RT-speeding follows a developmental trajectory, with little evidence for behavioral enhancement before age 9, but that near full maturity is reached by age 16 [26],[34]. Moreover, in these developmental studies, behavioral performance was shown to benefit from MSI at the single participant level for 95% of neurotypical participants aged 11-16, and 100% of participants aged 13-16. This relatively protracted developmental trajectory of MSI behavioral facilitation is consistently seen across laboratories [35],[36]. Here we used this behavioral approach to assay multisensory function in three boys with NPC -- two adolescents (14 years, 7 months & 14 years, 5 months old) and one younger boy (11 years, 1 month) -- comparing their performance to that of 16 neurotypical adolescent boys aged 13-15, and 19 neurotypical boys aged 10-13, respectively.

Methods

Participants

Two adolescent boys with NPC (14 years, 7 months & 14 years, 5 months of age respectively) and one 11 year old boy with NPC (11 years, 1 month) participated in the study. NPC was clinically diagnosed by metabolic specialists and confirmed via genetic testing. Participants were administered the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (WASI-II) The WASI-II is a short and reliable measure of intelligence that assesses general intellectual functioning. All four subtests were used: Vocabulary, Block Design, Similarities, and Matrix Reasoning. Vocabulary measures the individual’s expressive vocabulary, verbal knowledge, and fund of information. Block Design measures spatial visualization, visual-motor coordination, and abstract conceptualization. The Similarities subtest measures verbal concept formation, abstract verbal reasoning ability, and general intellectual ability. Matrix Reasoning measures non-verbal fluid reasoning and general intellectual ability. Scores are reported as a Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), a Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI), and a Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ), which represents performance on all 4 subtests.

The three NPC patients were within the mild to moderately impaired range and moderately to severely impaired range (Patient 1: FSIQ = 76, VCI = 82, PRI = 74; Patient 2: FSIQ = 62, VCI = 69, PRI = 58; Patient 3: FSIQ = 63, VCI = 72, PRI = 56). Scores on each subtest of the WASI-II are detailed in Table 1. The two older patients exhibited mild high-frequency hearing loss and one of the older patients as well as the younger one had lower than average visual acuity. It is important to emphasize that both auditory and visual stimuli used in the experiment were well above their detectability thresholds. The reader is referred to Table 2 for more comprehensive phenotypic descriptions of each of the three NPC participants.

Table 1 Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence scores
Table 2 Clinical impressions

Thirty-five neurotypical boys also participated in this study. Sixteen adolescent boys aged 13-15 served as an age-matched control group for the two older patients. Nineteen boys aged 10-12 served as an age-matched control group for the younger patient. Participants were screened for neurological and psychiatric disorders, as well as other major medical conditions. These data were partially reported in a pair of previous studies [26],[34]. Participants were also administered the WASI-II and Full Scale IQ (FSIQ), Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), and Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI) scores were obtained, which for these groups were in the average or high average range (Older group mean (standard deviation - SD): FSIQ = 113 (12), VCI = 104 (14), PRI = 110 (12); Younger group: FSIQ = 113 (14), VCI = 108 (12), PRI = 113 (13)). Audiometric evaluation confirmed that all participants had within-normal-limits hearing thresholds. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Before entering into the study, informed written consent was obtained from the children's parents, and verbal or written assent was obtained from children. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at The Albert Einstein College of Medicine and were in accordance with the tenets for the responsible conduct of human research laid out in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Paradigm & task

Stimuli

Auditory alone

A 1000-Hz tone (duration 60 ms; 75 dB SPL; rise/fall time 5 ms) was presented from a single Hartman Multimedia JBL Duet speaker located centrally atop the computer monitor from which the visual stimulus was presented.

Visual alone

A red disc with a diameter of 3.2 cm (subtending 1.5° in diameter at a viewing distance of 122 cm) appearing on a black background was presented on a Liquid Crystal Display (LCD) monitor (Dell Ultrasharp 1704FTP, 60Hz refresh rate) for 60 ms. The disc was located 0.4 cm superior to central fixation along the vertical meridian (0.9° at a viewing distance of 122 cm). A small cross marked the point of central fixation on the monitor.

Auditory and visual simultaneous

The “auditory-alone” and “visual-alone” conditions described above were presented simultaneously. The auditory and visual stimuli were presented in close spatial proximity, with the speaker placed atop the monitor in vertical alignment with the visual stimulus.

Procedures

Participants were seated in a dimly lit, sound-attenuated electrically shielded room (Industrial Acoustics Company, Bronx, New York) 122 cm from the monitor. They were given a response pad (Logitech Wingman Precision) and instructed to press a button with their right thumb as quickly as possible when they saw the red circle, heard the tone, or saw the circle and heard the tone together. The same response key was used for all 3 stimulus types. Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany CA) was used for stimulus delivery. This software ensures precise timing of stimulus presentation and is commonly used in neuroscience, psychophysics, and psychological experiments. It takes into account the refresh rate of the computer monitor when presenting visual stimuli. In this experiment, stimulus delivery in the multisensory condition was triggered by the onset of the visual stimulus. All 3 stimulus types were presented with equal probability and in random order in blocks of 100 trials. Inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) varied randomly between 1000 and 3000 (ms) according to a uniform (square wave) distribution. Participants completed a minimum of 8 blocks, with most completing 10. Breaks were encouraged between blocks to help maintain concentration and reduce restlessness or fatigue (these methods are also presented in detail in Brandwein et al [26],[34] and Molholm et al [21]).

Interrogating the race model

To test the race model, we first calculated the cumulative probability of reaction times across the three stimulus types (audio-alone, visual-alone, and audio-visual) for each of the participants. The range of RTs accepted was determined at the individual participant level with the slowest and fastest 2.5% of trials excluded. Using a 95% cutoff to define the time window for acceptable trials rather than an absolute cutoff value allowed us to more accurately capture the range of RTs for each participant, an important factor in calculating the race model (described below). The RT distribution was then divided into quantiles from the 5th to the 100th percentile in increments of 5%. For any RT latency, t, the race model holds when this CP value is less than or equal to the sum of the CP from each of the unisensory conditions. Conversely, the race-model is said to be violated if the CP for any audiovisual RT latency is larger than that predicted by the race model (the sum of the unisensory CPs) at any quantile. Violations were expected to occur in the first third of the distribution (i.e. the quantiles containing the fastest RTs at the lower end of the RT range) because this is when interactions between visual and auditory inputs would result in the fulfillment of a response criterion before either input alone could satisfy the same criterion [31]. At the individual level, a participant was said to have shown race model violation if the CP of his RT to the audiovisual stimulus was larger than that predicted by the race model at any quantile within the first third of the distribution. In order to more easily interpret results from the race model test, a Miller inequality value can be computed, both at the individual and group levels, by subtracting the CP predicted by the race model from the CP of the multisensory condition. Any positive “Miller values” indicate race model violation and RT speeding that cannot be accounted for by probability summation or by the ‘redundant signals effect’.

Results

Behavioral performance - reaction times & hit rates

The neurotypical group had a higher percentage of hits (correctly pressing the button to stimulus presentations) than the NPC participants. Hit rates are presented in Table 3. The current report was primarily concerned with the speed of responding. Overall, neurotypical participants were faster than the NPC patients (Table 4 and Figure 1). In order to examine RT variability independent of mean RT differences between the groups and between experimental conditions, the coefficient of variation (CV) was calculated for auditory, visual, and audiovisual conditions for each individual participant. The CV for the older patients fell within the neurotypical distribution or overlapped with individual neurotypical outliers. The CV for the younger patient fell outside (but close) to the neurotypical distribution; however there were also younger neurotypical controls that were more variable than this younger patient (see Additional file 1). What's more, in both neurotypical age-groups, variability was greatest for the auditory condition and did not differ significantly between the two other conditions. Observationally, the CV for individual NPC patients did not appear to differ substantially across experimental conditions. Nonparametric tests revealed no significant differences in RT variability based on stimulus type. Thus, increased variability in the multisensory condition should not affect the race model analysis presented below (for a Discussion see [37]). Detailed analyses and figures related to CV are provided in Additional file 1.

Table 3 Hit rates
Table 4 Reaction times
Figure 1
figure 1

Reaction time box and whisker plots. The plots show the distributions of mean RT values for 13-15 year olds (Panel A) and for 10-12 year olds (Panel B), for the two unisensory (Audio and Visual) and the multisensory (Audiovisual) conditions. The red symbols represent the mean RT values for each of the Niemann-Pick type C participants and the black crosses represent mean RT values for individual outliers from the neurotypical groups.

A repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant effect of stimulus type on RTs for both the older F(2,30) = 12.1, p < .001 and younger F(2,36) = 91.4, p < .001 neurotypical groups. Follow-up protected t-tests confirm a speeding up of RTs for the multisensory condition for the older neurotypical group (Audio vs. AV - t(15) = 3.4, p < .01; Visual vs. AV - t(15) = 5.0, p <. 01; Audio vs. Visual - t(15) = -.31, p = .76) and for the younger neurotypical group (Audio vs. AV - t(18) = 10.4, p < .01, Visual vs. AV- t(18) = 12.4, p < .01). Additionally, the younger group had significantly faster RTs to the auditory condition as compared to the visual condition, t(18) = -3.1, p < .01.

As our NPC sample contained only 3 participants, we performed a nonparametric bootstrapping procedure at the level of the individual participant data to compare RTs across the three sensory conditions (Figure 2). For each NPC patient, we compared the RTs in each of the unisensory conditions against the multisensory RTs, as well as against each other. The observed differences in mean RT between Audio vs. AV, Visual vs. AV, and Audio vs. Visual were compared with reference distributions of differences that were derived by iteratively randomizing (10,000 times) between the two original RT distributions - i.e. individual-subject single trial RTs for 1) Audio and AV, 2) Visual and AV, and 3) Audio and Visual. A two-tailed threshold of p <0.05 was used to define significance. The p value for a randomization test was calculated from the proportion of values in the reference difference distribution that exceeded the actual observed difference. In other words, we created a randomized sample distribution of possible reaction time differences, and sought to determine the likelihood that the actually observed differences (either speeding up or slowing down) were due to chance. There was no significant difference between auditory and visual RTs for the older NPC participants. The younger participant (Participant 3) showed significantly faster RTs in the visual condition compared to the auditory (p = .015). A significant speeding up was noted in the multisensory condition relative to the visual condition (p < .01), but not the auditory condition, for Participant 1. This was likely driven by the response to the auditory stimulus as the speeding up is only significant in the AV vs. V comparison. A significant speeding up was noted in the multisensory condition relative to the auditory condition (p < .05), but not the visual condition, for Participant 3. Again, this was likely driven by the response to the visual stimulus as the speeding up is only significant in the AV vs. A comparison. A significant speeding up in the multisensory condition compared to both unisensory conditions (p's < .01) was noted for Participant 2, indicating the presence of a Redundant Signals Effect. These tests, however, do not take into account facilitation due to multisensory interactions, which will be tested below using the race model calculation.

Figure 2
figure 2

Nonparametric randomization plots for the individual-participant reaction time data for each Niemann-Pick type C patient. RTs in each of the unisensory conditions were compared against the multisensory RTs (middle and right columns), and against each other (left column). The observed differences in mean RT between Audio vs. AV, Visual vs. AV, and Audio vs. Visual (red line) were compared with reference distributions of differences that were derived by iteratively randomizing (10,000 times) between the two original data sets (i.e. individual-subject single trial RTs for 1) Audio and AV, 2) Visual and AV, and 3) Audio and Visual). Significant differences (p < .05) are indicated by an asterisk. The findings are mixed. In two of the three patients, any apparent multisensory speeding is not significantly faster than the faster of the two unisensory responses. However, in one of the patients (Participant 2), RTs to the AV condition are significantly faster compared to both unisensory inputs. This particular patient is showing strong evidence for the so-called redundant sensory effect, but this speeding does not violate the race model.

If motor difficulties alone were to account for the larger variance in RTs and lower hit rates in the NPC participants, one would expect these to occur at the same probability across all three experimental conditions, which is not the case in this sample. Deficits in motor response do not account for the differential effect noted in 2 of the patients across the unisensory and multisensory conditions. The two NPC adolescents had faster RTs and a higher percentage of hits in the multisensory conditions compared to the unisensory. To probe the nature of this speeding up and assess whether the patients may be benefitting from an integrative process, we applied a test for multisensory integration effects (i.e. testing the race model). In this test a within-individual analysis is employed, thus accommodating the between group differences already noted.

Multisensory integration effects - race model

None of the three NPC participants showed any evidence of race model violation. Although in some cases, they showed faster RTs in the audiovisual condition (see above), this was not greater than could be accounted for by simple probability summation. In stark contrast, all of the neurotypical adolescents in our older sample of 13-15 year olds showed individual-level race model violation, suggesting that in this age group, multisensory integration reliably improves behavioral performance under these conditions. For the 11 year old NPC patient, an additional cutoff criterion was applied to his RT data before computing the race model. Unlike the rest of our sample, even after excluding the fastest 2.5% of RTs, this participant still had several anticipatory RTs that would be physiologically impossible (i.e. response latencies in the 40-100 ms range). These anticipatory responses were evenly distributed across all stimulus conditions (12% of the Audio trials, 13.5% Visual trials, and 10% of the AV trials). In order to eliminate any button presses that weren't directly in response to the stimulus, a hard cutoff criteria of 150ms was employed in his case, as it is generally agreed upon that shorter response latencies indicate actions that were initiated before the stimulus onset [38]-[44]. In the younger sample of 10-12 year olds, 16 of the 19 participants showed individual-level race model violation. Figure 3 depicts the CP distributions of reaction times for each of the experimental conditions -- audio-alone (blue), visual-alone (green), audiovisual (red), and the race model prediction (using the sum of the CPs of the unisensory responses (teal). Data for the three NPC boys are depicted across the top row. Across the middle row, data from three neurotypical individuals whose RT variability closely matched that of the NPC children are plotted for comparison. Despite similar RT variance, each of these neurotypicals shows race model violation. The bottom row shows data from an additional three neurotypical boys, where RT mean has been matched to each of the NPC boys. Again, all 3 neurotypicals show clear race model violation.

Figure 3
figure 3

Cumulative reaction time (RT) probability distributions. The cumulative probability of RTs for the three Niemann-Pick type C patients (top row) are compared to those of six neurotypical boys. The three age-matched comparison subjects depicted along the middle are chosen for their highly similar RT variance. The bottom row depicts three age-matched controls chosen for their highly similar mean RTs to those of the NPC boys. In the case of all six neurotypical controls, the observed cumulative RT distribution to the multisensory audio-visual condition (red curve) is faster than the prediction of the race model (cyan curve), indicating race model violation (i.e. multisensory integration). In none of the three NPC cases is this pattern observed.

Figure 4A & 4C depict plots of “Miller inequality” values which were obtained by subtracting the CP predicted by the race model from the CP for the multisensory condition. Positive values represent race model violation. Here it can be seen that the traces representing the two older NPC participants (4A- red) are never positive, whereas the trace representing the older neurotypical controls (blue) is positive for the quantiles representing the fastest ~30% of RTs. The shape of this Miller inequality function for neurotypical controls is highly similar to those reported in similar studies examining audio-visual integration [26],[34]. The Miller inequality plot for the younger neurotypical controls (Figure 4C- blue) closely approximates the pattern seen in the older children, albeit more immature. In the younger NPC participant, no race model violation is noted and the shape of his Miller inequality plot has the same atypical pattern noted in the two older NPC participants. Figure 4B & 4D depict box and whisker plots, which offer an additional representation of these data. Here the box and whiskers (blue rectangles with black bars) represent the Miller inequality values for all of the participants in the neurotypical group for the first six quantiles, which is the section of the RT distribution containing the fastest responses and also where race model violations are expected and seen in the neurotypical group (shaded area in Figure 4A & 4C). The small red shapes (squares and circles) represent the Miller inequality values for each NPC participant at these quantiles. This plot clearly shows that all three NPC boys fall completely outside the normal distribution between the second and sixth quantiles (10th, 15th, 20th, 25th, and 30th percentiles). Although race model violation is seen from the first quantile onward for the neurotypical participants, it is not necessarily seen for all participants at the exact same quantiles. That is to say that some participants will show race violation sooner than others and some will continue to show race model violation for several quantiles while the effect for others will dissipate more quickly. These effects, however, are generally seen in first third of the CP distribution as interactions between auditory and visual stimuli are likely to occur during these shorter latencies and so here we focus on the first 5 quantiles of this distribution. Further, we note that multisensory facilitation, as evidenced by race model violation (i.e. Miller inequality value greater than 0) was noted at the individual participants level for all 16 of the 13-15 year old neurotypical controls. For the 10-12 year olds, an age in which multisensory integration is still emerging and somewhat immature [45],[46], individual-level race model violation was seen for 16 out of 19 (84%) neurotypical controls. The NPC participants, on the other hand, failed to violate the race model at any point along the CP distribution. This lack of race model violation is especially striking for the older NPC participants as mean RT values for these NPC participants fall well within the neurotypical distribution in the case of one of the NPC patients, and overlaps with neurotypical outliers for the other patient (Figure 1A). This suggests a true multisensory deficit in that AV gains are accounted for by probability summation and there are no clear overall unisensory deficits contributing to this finding. For the younger participant, this is harder to say as his mean RTs for the auditory and the AV conditions fall slightly outside the neurotypical distribution. Nonetheless, the gains noted in his case can be adequately explained without evoking multisensory interactions as they are no greater than that predicted by probability summation.

Figure 4
figure 4

Race model test and Miller value spread. A & C). Race model plots depict the Miller value for the neurotypical groups (blue curves) and the Niemann-Pick type C patients (red curves). Values above zero indicate race model violation, which are evident in both the older neurotypicals (N = 16; Panel A) and the younger neurotypicals (N = 19; Panel B), but not in the NPC patients. The shape of the Miller inequality plot observed in the NPC patients is highly atypical and consistent across all three patients. B & D). Box and Whisker plots depict the spread of Miller values for the first 6 RT quantiles for the neurotypical group and the single subject Miller values for each of the NPC adolescents (red circles and red squares). This plot depicts the spread of Miller values for approximately 99% of both neurotypical groups, with the box representing 50% of the data, the whiskers representing the top 25 and bottom 25 percent, and the horizontal bisecting line representing the median Miller value for each neurotypical group at that quantile. It can be seen that all three NPC patients (red shapes) fall outside of the distribution of Miller values for their age-matched neurotypical group. Multisensory facilitation at the individual participant level was noted in all 16 of the 13-15 year olds and in 16 of 19 of the 11-12 year old neurotypicals.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine multisensory processes in NPC. The observed lack of race model violation in NPC suggests compromised connectivity between auditory and visual areas of the brain, possibly at both sub-cortical and cortical levels. It is likely that these inter-sensory connections develop very early in life, strengthen across childhood, and stabilize during adolescence [26],[34],[47],[48].Understanding when exactly during the progression of NPC that MSI becomes compromised will require further investigation and will be crucial to maximizing the clinical usefulness of this measure in the NPC population. Two possible scenarios are that; 1) MSI-induced behavioral facilitation never quite reaches “healthy” levels in these individuals or 2) that like many of the other symptoms exhibited in this population, NPC patients experience a degradation of MSI function with progression of the disease state. In either case, this metric of MSI presents a behavioral marker against which to measure improved neurocognitive function due to experimental treatment interventions.

In terms of everyday functioning, an obvious question is what impact deficits in multisensory processing will have on the abilities of NPC children to effectively navigate their environment. For example, effective MSI leads to improved speech perception when a listener has the benefit of watching the facial articulations of a speaker, especially if the fidelity of the auditory input is affected by noisy background environmental conditions [17],[22],[23],[49],[50]. Thus, one implication is that these children may find communication more difficult in challenging multi-speaker scenarios, not uncommon in classrooms or other social settings. MSI is also vital to more basic functions, such as maintaining balance through visuo-vestibular and visual-somatosensory integration [15] and in speeded orienting to reliable multisensory events, whether it be for object identification or cueing initiation of approach/avoidance behaviors [16],[18]-[21],[24]. A more comprehensive understanding of the multisensory integration abilities of these children is clearly called for, and it will be of significant interest to assess the underlying neurophysiology in turn [51],[52].

Another obvious outcome of the current study is that the NPC children show basic motor deficits. While it is true that there are neurotypical participants who are as slow to respond to unisensory inputs, and others who show similarly high variance in RTs, no neurotypical children show the poor response rates we see in the NPC children. Simply put, the NPC children are slow, variable and inaccurate and this triumvirate of issues clearly points to fundamental sensory-motor issues. That said, we do not believe that the MSI deficits observed here are primarily due to these issues, since these issues apply equally to all the experimental conditions (both unisensory and multisensory; also see Additional file 1). As the race model analysis is conducted at the individual participant level, where the cumulative probability distributions are calculated for each participant and within-subject RTs are compared to determine the multisensory benefit, general motor delays are accounted for. It could reasonably be asked, though, whether simple tests of motor speed, variance and accuracy might not prove equally useful biomarkers for NPC. However, it bears re-emphasizing that while the NPC children do show these issues, their performance levels do not fall completely outside the normal distribution for these measures, whereas for the measures of multisensory integration, they clearly do.

It is worth pointing out that these children with NPC are, at some basic level, benefitting from multisensory stimulation, even if not in an integrative manner. The fact that mean RTs and hits are improved in some cases, even in the absence of significant multisensory integration, when patients are exposed to stimulation in two sensory streams is promising, especially in terms of sensory training. This may have implications for the development of assistive technologies used for communication, particularly during the more progressed phases of the disease.

A natural question that arises is whether the multisensory deficit we observe in NPC can be meaningfully impacted through intervention. The landscape is actually quite promising in this regard since several studies now point to multisensory and unisensory gain with repeated training. These studies show that training can lead to improvement in MSI-dependent tasks such as speech-perception [53], that training can narrow the time window during which two sensory inputs are seen as “synchronous” and thus integrated [54], and that MSI networks can be engaged and enhanced in training activities where abstract stimuli are paired, such as specific sounds with abstract shapes, or musical tones with symbols [55],[56]. Work in animal models also supports the notion that sensory integration abilities can be impacted through practice with training-induced multisensory enhancement noted in both behavior and activity patterns at the single cell level in the superior colliculus, in both juvenile [57] and adult cats [58].

An obvious limitation of the current work is the relatively small cohort of three patients with NPC that we were able to test. Ideally, one would like to have greater numbers. However, the disease prevalence rate for NPC is estimated at 1-in-120,000 [6],[8],[59], so recruitment of larger populations is extremely challenging. It is worth emphasizing that the atypical multisensory integration pattern noted here is highly consistent across the 3 NPC patients in our sample and the findings are strengthened by comparison of these 3 patients to large existing datasets of neurotypical age-matched children. In all 3 cases, the performance metrics of the NPC patients fall completely outside the “normative” curve for MSI development.

Conclusions

This study uncovered clear multisensory deficits in three patients with NPC. The simple-to-acquire measures of multisensory response speed described here may prove to be useful endpoints against which to track disease progression and to assess the efficacy of therapeutic interventions. Specific environmental accommodations should be considered to address the potential impact of deteriorating multisensory mechanisms in these children.

Authors’ contributions

GA, SM, SUW and JJF conceived the study. GA and ABB coordinated data collection. JSB and GA conducted the primary data analyses. GA wrote the initial draft of the paper and all authors provided multiple rounds of input during the editorial process. The senior author, JJF, attests that all authors had full access to the data and that each author saw and approved the final submitted version of this manuscript.

Additional file

References

  1. Vanier MT, Duthel S, Rodriguez-Lafrasse C, Pentchev P, Carstea ED: Genetic heterogeneity in Niemann-Pick C disease: a study using somatic cell hybridization and linkage analysis. Am J Hum Genet. 1996, 58: 118-125.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Vanier MT, Suzuki K: Recent advances in elucidating Niemann-Pick C disease. Brain Pathol. 1998, 8: 163-174. 10.1111/j.1750-3639.1998.tb00143.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Vanier MT: Biochemical studies in Niemann-Pick disease. I. Major sphingolipids of liver and spleen. Biochim Biophys Acta. 1983, 750: 178-184. 10.1016/0005-2760(83)90218-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Vanier MT: Lipid changes in Niemann-Pick disease type C brain: personal experience and review of the literature. Neurochem Res. 1999, 24: 481-489. 10.1023/A:1022575511354.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Zervas M, Dobrenis K, Walkley SU: Neurons in Niemann-Pick disease type C accumulate gangliosides as well as unesterified cholesterol and undergo dendritic and axonal alterations. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2001, 60: 49-64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Mengel E, Klunemann HH, Lourenco CM, Hendriksz CJ, Sedel F, Walterfang M, Kolb SA: Niemann-Pick disease type C symptomatology: an expert-based clinical description. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2013, 8: 166-10.1186/1750-1172-8-166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Garver WS, Francis GA, Jelinek D, Shepherd G, Flynn J, Castro G, Vockley CW, Coppock DL, Pettit KM, Heidenreich RA, Meany FJ: The National Niemann-Pick C1 disease database: report of clinical features and health problems. Am J Med Genet A. 2007, 143A: 1204-1211. 10.1002/ajmg.a.31735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Patterson MC, Hendriksz CJ, Walterfang M, Sedel F, Vanier MT, Wijburg F: Recommendations for the diagnosis and management of Niemann-Pick disease type C: an update. Mol Genet Metab. 2012, 106: 330-344. 10.1016/j.ymgme.2012.03.012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Wraith JE, Baumgartner MR, Bembi B, Covanis A, Levade T, Mengel E, Pineda M, Sedel F, Topcu M, Vanier MT, Widner H, Wijburg FA, Patterson MC: Recommendations on the diagnosis and management of Niemann-Pick disease type C. Mol Genet Metab. 2009, 98: 152-165. 10.1016/j.ymgme.2009.06.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Walkley SU, Suzuki K: Consequences of NPC1 and NPC2 loss of function in mammalian neurons. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2004, 1685: 48-62. 10.1016/j.bbalip.2004.08.011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Liu B, Turley SD, Burns DK, Miller AM, Repa JJ, Dietschy JM: Reversal of defective lysosomal transport in NPC disease ameliorates liver dysfunction and neurodegeneration in the npc1-/- mouse. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009, 106: 2377-2382. 10.1073/pnas.0810895106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Sarna JR, Larouche M, Marzban H, Sillitoe RV, Rancourt DE, Hawkes R: Patterned Purkinje cell degeneration in mouse models of Niemann-Pick type C disease. J Comp Neurol. 2003, 456: 279-291. 10.1002/cne.10522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Aqul A, Liu B, Ramirez CM, Pieper AA, Estill SJ, Burns DK, Liu B, Repa JJ, Turley SD, Dietschy JM: Unesterified cholesterol accumulation in late endosomes/lysosomes causes neurodegeneration and is prevented by driving cholesterol export from this compartment. J Neurosci. 2011, 31: 9404-9413. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1317-11.2011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Davidson CD, Ali NF, Micsenyi MC, Stephney G, Renault S, Dobrenis K, Ory DS, Vanier MT, Walkley SU: Chronic cyclodextrin treatment of murine Niemann-Pick C disease ameliorates neuronal cholesterol and glycosphingolipid storage and disease progression. PLoS One. 2009, 4: e6951-10.1371/journal.pone.0006951.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Bair WN, Kiemel T, Jeka JJ, Clark JE: Development of multisensory reweighting for posture control in children. Exp Brain Res. 2007, 183: 435-446. 10.1007/s00221-007-1057-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Foxe JJ: Multisensory integration: frequency tuning of audio-tactile integration. Curr Biol. 2009, 19: R373-R375. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.029.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Foxe JJ, Molholm S, Del Bene VA, Frey HP, Russo NN, Blanco DB, Saint-Amour D, Ross L: Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence.Cereb Cortex 2013, [Epub ahead of print].,

  18. Foxe JJ, Schroeder CE: The case for feedforward multisensory convergence during early cortical processing. Neuroreport. 2005, 16: 419-423. 10.1097/00001756-200504040-00001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Molholm S, Martinez A, Shpaner M, Foxe JJ: Object-based attention is multisensory: co-activation of an object's representations in ignored sensory modalities. Eur J Neurosci. 2007, 26: 499-509. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05668.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Molholm S, Ritter W, Javitt DC, Foxe JJ: Multisensory visual-auditory object recognition in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. Cereb Cortex. 2004, 14: 452-465. 10.1093/cercor/bhh007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Molholm S, Ritter W, Murray MM, Javitt DC, Schroeder CE, Foxe JJ: Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2002, 14: 115-128. 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00066-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Ross LA, Molholm S, Blanco D, Gomez-Ramirez M, Saint-Amour D, Foxe JJ: The development of multisensory speech perception continues into the late childhood years. Eur J Neurosci. 2011, 33: 2329-2337. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07685.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Ross LA, Saint-Amour D, Leavitt VM, Javitt DC, Foxe JJ: Do you see what I am saying? Exploring visual enhancement of speech comprehension in noisy environment. Cereb Cortex. 2007, 17: 1147-1153. 10.1093/cercor/bhl024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Stein BE, Meredith MA: Multisensory integration. Neural and behavioral solutions for dealing with stimuli from different sensory modalities. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1990, 608: 51-65. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48891.x. discussion 65-70

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Cappe C, Rouiller EM, Barone P: Cortical and Thalamic Pathways for Multisensory and Sensorimotor Interplay. The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes. Edited by: Murray MM, Wallace MT. 2012, CRC Press, Boca Raton (FL)

    Google Scholar 

  26. Brandwein AB, Foxe JJ, Russo NN, Altschuler TS, Gomes H, Molholm S: The development of audiovisual multisensory integration across childhood and early adolescence: a high-density electrical mapping study. Cereb Cortex. 2011, 21: 1042-1055. 10.1093/cercor/bhq170.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Harrington LK, Peck CK: Spatial disparity affects visual-auditory interactions in human sensorimotor processing. Exp Brain Res. 1998, 122: 247-252. 10.1007/s002210050512.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hughes HC, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Nozawa G, Fendrich R: Visual-auditory interactions in sensorimotor processing: saccades versus manual responses. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1994, 20: 131-153. 10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.131.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Maravita A, Bolognini N, Bricolo E, Marzi CA, Savazzi S: Is audiovisual integration subserved by the superior colliculus in humans?. Neuroreport. 2008, 19: 271-275. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f4f04e.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Murray MM, Molholm S, Michel CM, Heslenfeld DJ, Ritter W, Javitt DC, Schroeder CE, Foxe JJ: Grabbing your ear: rapid auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in low-level sensory cortices are not constrained by stimulus alignment. Cereb Cortex. 2005, 15: 963-974. 10.1093/cercor/bhh197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Miller J: Divided attention: evidence for coactivation with redundant signals. Cogn Psychol. 1982, 14: 247-279. 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90010-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Miller J: Timecourse of coactivation in bimodal divided attention. Percept Psychophys. 1986, 40: 331-343. 10.3758/BF03203025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Ulrich R, Miller J, Schroter H: Testing the race model inequality: an algorithm and computer programs. Behav Res Methods. 2007, 39: 291-302. 10.3758/BF03193160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Brandwein AB, Foxe JJ, Butler JS, Russo NN, Altschuler TS, Gomes H, Molholm S: The development of multisensory integration in high-functioning autism: high-density electrical mapping and psychophysical measures reveal impairments in the processing of audiovisual inputs. Cereb Cortex. 2013, 23: 1329-1341. 10.1093/cercor/bhs109.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Gori M, Del Viva M, Sandini G, Burr DC: Young children do not integrate visual and haptic form information. Curr Biol. 2008, 18: 694-698. 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.036.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Nardini M, Jones P, Bedford R, Braddick O: Development of cue integration in human navigation. Curr Biol. 2008, 18: 689-693. 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Otto TU, Mamassian P: Noise and correlations in parallel perceptual decision making. Curr Biol. 2012, 22: 1391-1396. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.031.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Brebner JT, Welford AT: Introduction: an Historical Background Sketch. 1980, Academic, New York

    Google Scholar 

  39. Fieandt K v, Huhtala A, Kullberg P, Saarl K: Personal Tempo and Phenomenal Time at Different Age Levels. 1956

    Google Scholar 

  40. Galton F: On instruments for (1) testing perception of differences of tint and for (2) determining reaction time. J Anthropol Inst. 1899, 19: 27-29.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Jevas S, Yan JH: The effect of aging on cognitive function: A preliminary quantitative review. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2001, 72: A49-A49.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Welford AT: Motor performance. 1977, New York, Van Nostrand Reinhold

    Google Scholar 

  43. Welford AT: Choice reaction time: Basic concepts. 1980, New York, Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  44. Welford AT: Reaction time, speed of performance, and age. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988, 515: 1-17. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb32958.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Barutchu A, Crewther DP, Crewther SG: The race that precedes coactivation: development of multisensory facilitation in children. Dev Sci. 2009, 12: 464-473. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00782.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Nardini M, Begus K, Mareschal D: Multisensory uncertainty reduction for hand localization in children and adults. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2013, 39: 773-787. 10.1037/a0030719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Wallace MT, Carriere BN, Perrault TJ, Vaughan JW, Stein BE: The development of cortical multisensory integration. J Neurosci. 2006, 26: 11844-11849. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3295-06.2006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Wallace MT, Stein BE: Early experience determines how the senses will interact. J Neurophysiol. 2007, 97: 921-926. 10.1152/jn.00497.2006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Ma WJ, Zhou X, Ross LA, Foxe JJ, Parra LC: Lip-reading aids word recognition most in moderate noise: a Bayesian explanation using high-dimensional feature space. PLoS One. 2009, 4: e4638-10.1371/journal.pone.0004638.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Saint-Amour D, De Sanctis P, Molholm S, Ritter W, Foxe JJ: Seeing voices: High-density electrical mapping and source-analysis of the multisensory mismatch negativity evoked during the McGurk illusion. Neuropsychologia. 2007, 45: 587-597. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.036.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Foxe JJ, Morocz IA, Murray MM, Higgins BA, Javitt DC, Schroeder CE: Multisensory auditory-somatosensory interactions in early cortical processing revealed by high-density electrical mapping. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2000, 10: 77-83. 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00024-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Foxe JJ, Wylie GR, Martinez A, Schroeder CE, Javitt DC, Guilfoyle D, Ritter W, Murray MM: Auditory-somatosensory multisensory processing in auditory association cortex: an fMRI study. J Neurophysiol. 2002, 88: 540-543.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Bernstein LE, Auer ET, Eberhardt SP, Jiang J: Auditory Perceptual Learning for Speech Perception Can be Enhanced by Audiovisual Training. Front Neurosci. 2013, 7: 34-10.3389/fnins.2013.00034.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Powers AR, Hevey MA, Wallace MT: Neural correlates of multisensory perceptual learning. J Neurosci. 2012, 32: 6263-6274. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6138-11.2012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Butler AJ, James KH: Active learning of novel sound-producing objects: motor reactivation and enhancement of visuo-motor connectivity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013, 25: 203-218. 10.1162/jocn_a_00284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Paraskevopoulos E, Kuchenbuch A, Herholz SC, Pantev C: Multisensory integration during short-term music reading training enhances both uni- and multisensory cortical processing. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014, 26: 2224-2238. 10.1162/jocn_a_00620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Stein BE, Rowland BA: Organization and plasticity in multisensory integration: early and late experience affects its governing principles. Prog Brain Res. 2011, 191: 145-163. 10.1016/B978-0-444-53752-2.00007-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Yu L, Rowland BA, Xu J, Stein BE: Multisensory plasticity in adulthood: cross-modal experience enhances neuronal excitability and exposes silent inputs. J Neurophysiol. 2013, 109: 464-474. 10.1152/jn.00739.2012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Vanier MT: Niemann-Pick disease type C. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2010, 5: 16-10.1186/1750-1172-5-16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We extend our deep appreciation to the families involved in this research for their time, patience, and care. The Human Clinical Phenotyping Core, where the children in this study were clinically evaluated, is a facility of the Rose F. Kennedy Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (IDDRC) which is funded through a center grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD P30 HD071593). Ongoing support of the Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory is provided through a grant from the Sheryl and Daniel R. Tishman Charitable Foundation. Support for this work was provided in part by the Support of Accelerated Research of NPC Disease foundation (SOAR-NPC). We thank Greg Peters and Emmett Foxe for assistance with data collection and Drs. Juliana Bates and Zonya Mitchell for help with clinical testing.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John J Foxe.

Additional information

Competing interests

All authors declare no competing interests, financial or otherwise, that could have impacted the work reported herein.

Electronic supplementary material

13023_2014_149_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional file 1: Variability analysis. Figure S1. Coefficient of Variation Spread. Table S1. Coefficient of Variation. (DOCX 58 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Andrade, G.N., Molholm, S., Butler, J.S. et al. Atypical multisensory integration in Niemann-Pick type C disease – towards potential biomarkers. Orphanet J Rare Dis 9, 149 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-014-0149-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-014-0149-x

Keywords